Development of Drawing Test
We need to present evidence of growth for transfer from Ric Cognitive Approach® training to measures of IQ and intellectual development for our students. There are two measurement instruments we use for such facilitation, namely the standardised IQ tests and Drawing tests. Regrettably, many parents do not have the knowledge on the cognitive value of drawing tests, and their association with children’s IQ.
The study of children’s drawings covers different concepts and is thus used as an instrument to estimate various types of functions. In particular, children’s drawings are very often linked to their IQ. Developed by Goodenough in 1926, Draw-A-Man test was the first scoring system to measure intelligence through children’s drawings.
Early Findings of Children’s Drawings*
Children do not draw what they see, but rather their internal representations during early attempts to draw; and for Luquet, this is termed intellectual realism and is differentiated from the adult’s visual realism. Early studies of children’s drawing started over a century ago. Two major discoveries were made between 1900 and 1915. Firstly, it was found that the progression of children’s drawings is constant. Secondly, it was found that less intellectually gifted children tend to draw less complex pictures. In addition, the progression of children’s drawings was found closely related to their mental age and not to their actual chronological age.
Imagery Drawing Test Research at The University of Manchester*
In 2011, our founder, Ric performed a correlation study examining the relationship between drawing test scores and IQ. Children’s nonverbal intelligence was measured using three different instruments. Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices (CPM), the Goodenough-Harris Draw-A-Person test (DAP) and Imagery Drawing Test (IDT). 684 children between the ages of 7 and 11 from a public elementary school participated in the study. The results revealed strong correlation between DAP and IDT scores. IDT scores were modestly associated with CPM scores; DAP scores to CPM scores were, likewise, modestly associated. Results showed that IDT was the best predictor variable for both DAP and CPM, thereby indicating IDT as a highly feasible alternative drawing test to the traditional DAP for measuring children’s IQ.
Latest Development of Drawing Test
In 2014, there was heavy media coverage on the topic of children’s drawings and their IQ. Researchers from King’s College London conducted a 10-year study on 7,752 pairs of twins to find out the relationship between children’s drawings and their IQ. At the end of the study, they concluded that children’s drawings predict children’s IQ as well as their IQ a decade later.
Children’s mental representations through drawings can provide us with some ideas about their IQ. However, it must not be the only means of measure and should be used in conjunction with other forms of assessment.
Links
- http://www.bbc.com/news/education-28852471
- http://www.kcl.ac.uk/ioppn/news/records/2014/August/Childrens-drawings-indicate-later-intelligence.aspx
- http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2014/08/140818204114.htm
- http://www.medicaldaily.com/drawing-test-4-years-old-may-predict-intelligence-14-dont-worry-if-your-child-isnt-next-da-298582
- http://www.telegraph.co.uk/news/science/science-news/11041402/Childrens-drawings-could-be-a-clue-to-their-intelligence-study-finds.html
References*
Abell, S.C., Heiberger, A.M., & Johnson, J.E. (1994). Cognitive evaluations of young adults using human figure drawings: An empirical investigation of two methods. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 50, 900-905.
Abell, S.C., Von Briesen, P.D., & Watz, L.S. (1996). Intellectual evaluations of children using human figure drawings: An empirical investigation of two methods. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 52, 67-74.
Abell, S.C., Wood, W., & Liebman, S.J. (2001). Children’s human figure drawings as measures of intelligence: The comparative validity of three scoring systems. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 19, 204-215.
Aikman, K.G., Belter, R.W., & Finch, A.J. (1992). Human figure drawings: Validity in assessing intellectual level and academic achievement. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 48, 114-120.
Bekhit, N.S., Thomas, G.V., & Jolley, R.P. (2005). The use of drawing for psychological assessment in Britain: Survey findings. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 78, 205-217.
Cherney, I.D., Seiwert, C.S., Dickey, T.M., & Flichtbeil, J.D. (2006). Children’s drawings: A mirror to their minds. Educational Psychology, 26, 127-142.
Cox, M.V. (1993). Children’s drawings of the human figure. East Sussex: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Cox, M.V., Koyasu, M., Hiranuma, H., & Perara, J. (2001). Children’s human figure drawings in the UK and Japan: The effects of age, sex and culture. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19, 275-292.
Cox, M.V., & Parkin, C.E. (1986). Young children’s human figure drawing: Cross-sectional ang longitudinal studies. Educational Psychology, 6, 353-368.
Daglioglu, H.E., Calisandemir, F., Alemdar, M., & Kangal, S.B. (2010). Examination of human figure drawings by gifted and normally developed children at preschool period. Elementary Education Online, 9, 31-43.
Di Leo, J.H. (1973). Children’s drawings as diagnostic aids. New York: Brunner/Mazel Publishers.
Edwards, B. (2001). The new drawing on the right side of the brain. London: Harper Collins Publishers.
Harris, D.B. (1963). Children’s drawings as measures of intellectual maturity. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Jolley, R.P. (2010). Children and pictures: Drawing and understanding. West Sussex: Wiley-Blackwell.
Klepsch, M., & Logie, L. (1982). Children draw and tell: An introduction to the projective uses of children’s human figure drawings. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Oakland, T., & Dowling, L. (1983). The Draw-A-Person Test: Validity properties for nonbiased assessment. Learning Disability Quarterly, 6, 526-534.
Perets-Dubrovsky, S., Kaveh, M., Deutsh-Castel, T., Cohen, A., & Tirosh, E. (2010). The human figure drawing as related to Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Journal of Child Neurology, 25, 689-693.
Reisman, J.M., & Yamokoski, T. (1973). Can intelligence be estimated from drawings of a man? Journal of School Psychology, 11, 239-244.
Rosa, H.R. (2008). Human figure drawings’ validity for Goodenough-Harris Test and Koppitz Maturity Indicators in Sao Paulo children’s. Bolitim De Psicologia, 128, 1-14.
Scott, L.H. (1981). Measuring intelligence with the Goodenough-Harris Drawing Test. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 483-505.
Ter Laak, J., De Goede, M., Aleva, A., & Van Rijswijk, P. (2005). The Draw-A-Person test: An indicator of children’s cognitive and socioemotional adaptation? The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 166, 77-93.
Van Meerbeke, A.V., Sandoval-Garcia, C., Ibanez, M., Talero-Gutierrez, C., Fiallo, D., & Halliday, K. (2011). Validation study on human figure drawing test in a Colombian school children population. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 14, 464-477.
Willcock, E., Imuta, K., & Hayne, H. (2011). Children’s human figure drawings do not measure intellectual ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.04.013.
Williams Jr., T.O., Fall, A.M., Eaves, R.C., & Groves, S.W. (2006). The reliability of scores for the Draw-A-Person intellectual ability test for children, adolescents, and adults. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 24, 137-144.
* Copyright © 2011 Ric Chong. Extracted from our founder’s MEd PsyEd Dissertation at The University of Manchester, U.K.